Volume 19, Issue 5 p. 1947-1962
RESEARCH ARTICLE
Open Access

Temporal dynamics predict symptom onset and cognitive decline in familial frontotemporal dementia

David J. Whiteside

Corresponding Author

David J. Whiteside

Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, Cambridgeshire, UK

Cambridge University Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust, Cambridge, UK

Correspondence

David Whiteside, Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Herchel Smith Building, Cambridge Biomedical Campus, Cambridge CB2 0SZ, UK.

Email: [email protected]

Search for more papers by this author
Maura Malpetti

Maura Malpetti

Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, Cambridgeshire, UK

Search for more papers by this author
P. Simon Jones

P. Simon Jones

Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, Cambridgeshire, UK

Search for more papers by this author
Boyd C. P. Ghosh

Boyd C. P. Ghosh

Wessex Neurological Centre, University Hospital Southampton, Southampton, UK

Search for more papers by this author
Ian Coyle-Gilchrist

Ian Coyle-Gilchrist

Norfolk and Norwich University Hospital, Norwich, UK

Search for more papers by this author
John C. van Swieten

John C. van Swieten

Department of Neurology, Erasmus Medical Centre, Rotterdam, Netherlands

Search for more papers by this author
Harro Seelaar

Harro Seelaar

Department of Neurology, Erasmus Medical Centre, Rotterdam, Netherlands

Search for more papers by this author
Lize Jiskoot

Lize Jiskoot

Department of Neurology, Erasmus Medical Centre, Rotterdam, Netherlands

Search for more papers by this author
Barbara Borroni

Barbara Borroni

Centre for Neurodegenerative Disorders, Department of Clinical and Experimental Sciences, University of Brescia, Brescia, Italy

Search for more papers by this author
Raquel Sanchez-Valle

Raquel Sanchez-Valle

Alzheimer's Disease and Other Cognitive Disorders Unit, Neurology Service, Hospital Clínic, Institut d'Investigacións Biomèdiques August Pi I Sunyer, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain

Search for more papers by this author
Fermin Moreno

Fermin Moreno

Cognitive Disorders Unit, Department of Neurology, Donostia University Hospital, San Sebastian, Gipuzkoa, Spain

Neuroscience Area, Biodonostia Health Research Institute, San Sebastian, Gipuzkoa, Spain

Search for more papers by this author
Robert Laforce

Robert Laforce

CHU de Québec, and Faculté de Médecine, Département des Sciences Neurologiques, Clinique Interdisciplinaire de Mémoire, Université Laval, QC, Canada

Search for more papers by this author
Caroline Graff

Caroline Graff

Center for Alzheimer Research, Division of Neurogeriatrics, Department of Neurobiology, Care Sciences and Society, Bioclinicum, Karolinska Institutet, Solna, Sweden

Unit for Hereditary Dementias, Theme Aging, Karolinska University Hospital, Solna, Sweden

Search for more papers by this author
Matthis Synofzik

Matthis Synofzik

Department of Neurodegenerative Diseases, Hertie-Institute for Clinical Brain Research, Tübingen, Germany

Center of Neurology, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany

Search for more papers by this author
Daniela Galimberti

Daniela Galimberti

Fondazione IRCCS Ospedale Policlinico, Milan, Italy

Department of Biomedical, Surgical and Dental Sciences, University of Milan, Milan, Italy

Search for more papers by this author
Mario Masellis

Mario Masellis

Sunnybrook Health Sciences Centre, Sunnybrook Research Institute, University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada

Search for more papers by this author
Maria Carmela Tartaglia

Maria Carmela Tartaglia

Tanz Centre for Research in Neurodegenerative Diseases, University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada

Search for more papers by this author
Elizabeth Finger

Elizabeth Finger

Department of Clinical Neurological Sciences, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada

Search for more papers by this author
Rik Vandenberghe

Rik Vandenberghe

Laboratory for Cognitive Neurology, Department of Neurosciences, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium

Neurology Service, University Hospitals Leuven, Belgium

Leuven Brain Institute, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium

Search for more papers by this author
Alexandre de Mendonça

Alexandre de Mendonça

Faculty of Medicine, University of Lisbon, Lisbon, Portugal

Search for more papers by this author
Fabrizio Tagliavini

Fabrizio Tagliavini

Fondazione IRCCS Istituto Neurologico Carlo Besta, Milano, Italy

Search for more papers by this author
Chris R. Butler

Chris R. Butler

Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Medical Sciences Division, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK

Department of Brain Sciences, Imperial College London, London, UK

Search for more papers by this author
Isabel Santana

Isabel Santana

University Hospital of Coimbra (HUC), Neurology Service, Faculty of Medicine, University of Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal

Center for Neuroscience and Cell Biology, Faculty of Medicine, University of Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal

Search for more papers by this author
Isabelle Le Ber

Isabelle Le Ber

Paris Brain Institute – Institut du Cerveau – ICM, Inserm U1127, CNRS UMR 7225, AP-HP - Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Sorbonne Université, Paris, France

Centre de référence des démences rares ou précoces, IM2A, Département de Neurologie, AP-HP - Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Paris, France

Département de Neurologie, AP-HP - Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Paris, France

Search for more papers by this author
Alexander Gerhard

Alexander Gerhard

Division of Neuroscience and Experimental Psychology, Wolfson Molecular Imaging Centre, University of Manchester, Manchester, UK

Departments of Geriatric Medicine and Nuclear Medicine, University of Duisburg- Essen, Duisburg, Germany

Search for more papers by this author
Simon Ducharme

Simon Ducharme

Department of Psychiatry, McGill University Health Centre, McGill University, Montreal, Québec, Canada

Department of Neurology & Neurosurgery, McConnell Brain Imaging Centre, Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal, Canada

Search for more papers by this author
Johannes Levin

Johannes Levin

Neurologische Klinik, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, Munich, Germany

German Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Munich, Germany

Munich Cluster of Systems Neurology, Munich, Germany

Search for more papers by this author
Adrian Danek

Adrian Danek

Neurologische Klinik, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, Munich, Germany

Search for more papers by this author
Markus Otto

Markus Otto

Department of Neurology, University of Ulm, Ulm, Germany

Search for more papers by this author
Sandro Sorbi

Sandro Sorbi

Department of Neurofarba, University of Florence, Florence, Italy

IRCCS Fondazione Don Carlo Gnocchi, Florence, Italy

Search for more papers by this author
Florence Pasquier

Florence Pasquier

Univ Lille, Lille, France

Inserm 1172, Lille, France

CHU, CNR-MAJ, Labex Distalz, LiCEND Lille, Lille, France

Search for more papers by this author
Arabella Bouzigues

Arabella Bouzigues

Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, Dementia Research Centre UCL Institute of Neurology, Queen Square, London, UK

Search for more papers by this author
Lucy L. Russell

Lucy L. Russell

Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, Dementia Research Centre UCL Institute of Neurology, Queen Square, London, UK

Search for more papers by this author
Jonathan D. Rohrer

Jonathan D. Rohrer

Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, Dementia Research Centre UCL Institute of Neurology, Queen Square, London, UK

Search for more papers by this author
James B. Rowe

James B. Rowe

Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, Cambridgeshire, UK

Cambridge University Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust, Cambridge, UK

MRC Cognition and Brain Sciences Unit, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK

Search for more papers by this author
Timothy Rittman

Timothy Rittman

Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, Cambridgeshire, UK

Cambridge University Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust, Cambridge, UK

Search for more papers by this author
The GENFI consortium

The GENFI consortium

GENFI consortium authors listed in the appendix

Search for more papers by this author
First published: 15 November 2022
Citations: 2

Abstract

Introduction

We tested whether changes in functional networks predict cognitive decline and conversion from the presymptomatic prodrome to symptomatic disease in familial frontotemporal dementia (FTD).

Methods

For hypothesis generation, 36 participants with behavioral variant FTD (bvFTD) and 34 controls were recruited from one site. For hypothesis testing, we studied 198 symptomatic FTD mutation carriers, 341 presymptomatic mutation carriers, and 329 family members without mutations. We compared functional network dynamics between groups, with clinical severity and with longitudinal clinical progression.

Results

We identified a characteristic pattern of dynamic network changes in FTD, which correlated with neuropsychological impairment. Among presymptomatic mutation carriers, this pattern of network dynamics was found to a greater extent in those who subsequently converted to the symptomatic phase. Baseline network dynamic changes predicted future cognitive decline in symptomatic participants and older presymptomatic participants.

Discussion

Dynamic network abnormalities in FTD predict cognitive decline and symptomatic conversion.

Highlights

  1. We investigated brain network predictors of dementia symptom onset
  2. Frontotemporal dementia results in characteristic dynamic network patterns
  3. Alterations in network dynamics are associated with neuropsychological impairment
  4. Network dynamic changes predict symptomatic conversion in presymptomatic carriers
  5. Network dynamic changes are associated with longitudinal cognitive decline

1 INTRODUCTION

Neuropathological and structural changes accumulate over many years prior to the onset of symptoms in neurodegenerative diseases.1, 2 Understanding the timing and consequence of such changes for clinical syndromes is key to accounting for heterogeneity in progression and risk-stratifying asymptomatic individuals for preventative clinical trials. We have previously shown that functional network integrity is important in maintaining cognitive performance in individuals at risk of dementia,3, 4 with the corollary that loss of network integrity may herald symptom onset and predict cognitive decline. Genetic frontotemporal dementia (FTD) provides an opportunity to characterize functional networks throughout the course of the disease. Approximately one-third of patients with FTD have a family history in keeping with an autosomal dominant inheritance.5 Mutations in three genes account for the majority of these cases: chromosome 9 open reading frame 72 (C9orf72), granulin (GRN), and microtubule-associated protein tau (MAPT).5, 6 The resulting phenotypes are heterogeneous, with behavioral variant FTD (bvFTD) the most common clinical presentation.5

The coordination of neural activity across distributed spatial and temporal scales is dynamic.7-9 Such connectivity underpins cognition in health and is affected in psychiatric and neurodegenerative diseases.10-12 While canonical approaches to functional connectivity have averaged over the scan acquisition time, time-varying fluctuations in connectivity can also be captured by functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI).13-15 In the clinical syndromes of FTD, there are deficits in inhibitory and excitatory neurotransmitters required for network integration and segregation16 which we propose contribute to changes in temporal dynamics in the disease. Subtle changes in time-varying functional connectivity occur in presymptomatic mutation carriers,17 although their longitudinal significance and evolution into the symptomatic phase have not been studied.

We examined resting state brain dynamics in presymptomatic and symptomatic carriers of pathogenic mutation carriers in the Genetic Frontotemporal Initiative (GENFI) using fMRI to determine whether disruption to network dynamics predicts cognitive decline. We used hidden Markov modelling as a highly articulated data-driven approach to model the blood-oxygen-level-dependent signal of fMRI, an approach which posits the existence of a finite number of hidden states that describe the sequential evolution of observed data.15, 18

We investigated brain state dynamics using hidden Markov models (HMMs) with a two-stage approach to ensure replication and refine analytic choices. Hypothesis generation used a cohort of patients with mainly sporadic bvFTD and control participants recruited at the Cambridge Centre for FTD. We repeated the methodology in the GENFI, following preregistration of our cross-sectional analysis plan (https://osf.io/k64gh/wiki/home/), with the following hypotheses: (1) brain state dynamics differ between symptomatic mutation carriers and cognitively normal non-mutation carriers; (2) changes in network dynamics correlate with both neuropsychological deficits and carer assessed measures of impairment; (3) presymptomatic mutation carriers (versus non-mutation carriers) have abnormal network dynamics as a function of proximity to onset as denoted by age; and (4) altered network dynamics predict conversion from the presymptomatic to symptomatic phase and subsequent cognitive decline in gene mutation carriers.

RESEARCH IN CONTEXT

  1. Systematic Review: We reviewed published literature using traditional resources. Neuropathological and structural changes occur in dementia many years prior to the onset of symptoms. Assessing the onset of the symptomatic phase in those at high risk of dementia is clinically challenging, and neural correlates of conversion from the presymptomatic prodrome to symptomatic disease are not well characterized.
  2. Interpretation: Our results show that changes in the proportion of time spent in key brain networks in presymptomatic carriers of frontotemporal dementia mutations occur in the late presymptomatic phase. They are associated with conversion to symptomatic disease and subsequent cognitive decline.
  3. Future Directions: Dynamic brain network changes are a promising tool for stratification and prognostication in presymptomatic dementia, with implications for predicting outcome and risk-stratifying asymptomatic individuals for preventative clinical trials

2 MATERIALS AND METHODS

2.1 Participants

We used datasets from 36 participants with bvFTD and 34 healthy controls recruited at the Cambridge University Centre for Frontotemporal Dementia for hypothesis generation. Clinical assessment included the Addenbrooke's Cognitive Examination-Revised,19 Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE),20 Frontal Assessment Battery,21 and Cambridge Behavioral Inventory-Revised (CBI-R).22

The GENFI includes participants from 25 research sites across Europe and Canada. Participants were included if they were over 18 and had a known pathogenic mutation in MAPT, C9ORF72, GRN, or TBK1, or were a first degree relative of a mutation carrier. A total of 198 symptomatic mutation carriers, 341 asymptomatic mutation carriers, and 329 family members with usable fMRI (datafreeze 5) were included in this study. Clinicians classified mutation carriers as either symptomatic or presymptomatic, with participants deemed symptomatic if symptoms were present, were progressive in nature, and consistent with a diagnosis of an FTD-related degenerative disorder.

GENFI participants underwent a standardized assessment with clinical history, physical examination, neuropsychological assessment, and informant-based questionnaires.1 Severity of behavioral symptoms was assessed using the CBI-R. Neuropsychological tests included the Trail Making Tests, Digit Symbol Test, Backwards Digit Span, Letter and Category Fluency, a short version of the Boston Naming Test,23 and the MMSE. Assessments were repeated yearly or biannually, with longitudinal data up to 7 years post baseline visit.

2.2 Image acquisition and preprocessing

Image acquisition for the two cohorts and fMRI preprocessing have been published previously3, 4, 24 and are described in detail in the Supplementary Materials. Given the potential sensitivity of estimates of network dynamics to motion,25, 26 we excluded participants above previously defined thresholds for three data quality indices24 (maximum spike percentage,27 maximum framewise displacement,25 maximum spatial standard deviation of successive volume difference28). We excluded nine participants with bvFTD and 2 healthy controls from the Cambridge cohort, and 103 scans from 89 participants in the GENFI (20 non-carriers, 21 presymptomatic mutation carriers, 48 symptomatic participants). We performed an additional analysis excluding participants exceeding a mean framewise displacement threshold but included in the primary analysis (Supplementary Materials).

2.3 Hidden Markov models (HMMs)

We assessed network dynamics in both cohorts using HMMs.29 These models treat time series data as being generated from a finite number of unknown states. Each time point is therefore classified as being in a single state, although the assumption of state mutual exclusivity is adjusted through soft probabilistic inference. While the states and probability of transitioning between them are defined at the group level, a state time course can be estimated per participant.

We performed an independent component analysis (ICA) using MELODIC (fMRIB Software Library [FSL]) from preprocessed fMRI of all participants to allow comparison between cohorts. We chose a model order of 30 as a balance between excessive network fragmentation30 and predetermining HMM outputs. Six component maps were identified as artefact. Participant specific time courses for each component were generated by regression of the component maps into each subject's preprocessed fMRI. From standardized per participant non-artefactual component time courses a multivariate Gaussian HMM15 with six brain states was inferred for each cohort using the HMM-MAR toolbox (https://github.com/OHBA-analysis/HMM-MAR). All states shared a common covariance matrix.31 Model order was specified in registration prior to analyzing the GENFI dataset; it has previously been shown that robust behavioral inferences can be made through a six-state model.32 We repeated the analysis with ICA dimensionality determined automatically by MELODIC.

The temporal dynamics of HMM states can be characterized through a small set of metrics, namely switching rate (the frequency with which state transitions occur), fractional occupancy (the proportion of time a state is active), and the transition matrix consisting of transition probabilities (the chance of between-state transitions) and persistence probabilities (the chance of remaining in the same state). Mean activation maps were generated by weighting component maps by the mean of the state Gaussian distribution. For illustrative purposes we compared these maps with templates maps of canonical static functional networks,33 and performed an additional analysis of resting state connectivity to determine where connectivity changes occur in the GENFI cohort (see Supplementary Materials).

2.4 Statistical analyses

All statistical analyses were performed in R,34 with the exception of permutation testing using FSL's PALM (“Permutation Analysis of Linear Models”).35 P-values throughout were corrected across relevant tests for a false discovery rate of 5%, except permutation testing where family-wise error correction to 5% was performed across all tests and contrasts.

2.4.1 Descriptive statistics

We compared continuous variables between groups using independent sample t tests and categorical variables with the chi-square test.

2.4.2 Cambridge cohort

We compared fractional occupancy and switching rates between groups using a one-way analysis of covariance, with age and sex as covariates. For each participant we extracted matrices of the 36 transition and persistence probabilities. Given the interdependence of these probabilities, we assessed for group differences in a permutation test (5000 permutations) using FSL's PALM. Age and sex were included as covariates of no interest.

2.4.3 GENFI

In the GENFI data, cross-sectional analysis was performed using participants’ latest scan that passed motion thresholding, maximizing per-participant volume number. Differences in fractional occupancy rates and switching rates were assessed using mixed-effects linear models with diagnostic group as the main effect, age and sex as dependent variables, and scan site as a random intercept using the lme4 package.36 Significance values were calculated using the Satterwaithe estimate of effective degrees of freedom. Switching rates were adjusted to account for small differences in repetition time. Group differences in transition/persistence probabilities were calculated as per the Cambridge cohort.

For contrasts with clinical scores and longitudinal analysis we performed a principal component analysis on state fractional occupancies using the alfa.pca (alpha = 1) function from the Compositional package in R,37 followed by varimax rotation. We selected the number of components for analysis using MacArthur's "broken-stick" criterion.38

2.4.4 Network dynamics by age

In previous GENFI studies, mean family age at symptom onset has been used to estimate years until symptom onset, but only in MAPT mutations does this explain a significant proportion of variability in age of onset.39 Given that component scores did not differ by mutation type, we explored component scores by age as a marker of proximity to onset (comparing to family members without mutations, over a similar age range). We compared component scores and state occupancies between non-carriers and presymptomatic mutation carriers as a function of age, assessing the group by age (linear or quadratic) interaction.

2.4.5 Presymptomatic mutation carriers and neuropsychological correlates

We compared component scores in presymptomatic mutation carriers with pre-registered neuropsychological tests (Backwards Digit Span, Digit Symbol, Trail Making Test) as a function of age within a mixed-effects linear model.

2.4.6 Converters

Mutation carriers who were assessed during longitudinal follow-up as moving from the presymptomatic to symptomatic phase were classified as converters. We compared component scores, state occupancies, and neuropsychological scores between converters and non-converting presymptomatic mutation carriers at their latest presymptomatic scan in mixed-effects linear models with group as the main effect, age and sex as covariates, and scan site as a random variable.

2.4.7 Longitudinal cognitive data in symptomatic patients

A mixed linear model was used to calculate patient specific yearly rates of change in clinical and neurocognitive scores (MMSE, CBI-R, Backwards Digit Span, Digit Symbol, Trail Making Test B [TMTB]). Neurocognitive assessment was the dependent variable in the model, with years from baseline assessment as an independent variable and with estimation of intercept and slope (neurocognitive assessment ∼ time + (time | ID)). These models were calculated using all participants.

To assess whether baseline component scores predict neurocognitive decline, individual estimates of disease progression (slope) were taken to a second model as a dependent variable, with baseline component scores as an independent variable and baseline age, sex, and site as covariates of no interest.

2.4.8 Longitudinal cognition in presymptomatic mutation carriers

We repeated the two-step model for presymptomatic mutation carriers, additionally assessing the interaction between baseline component scores and age given that proximity to symptom onset increases the probability that small fluctuations in neurocognitive assessment are important.

3 RESULTS

3.1 Descriptive statistics

Demographic and clinical characteristics for the two cohorts for participants with a sub-motion threshold scan are included in Tables 1 and 2. In the Cambridge cohort no significant differences were observed in age or sex. In GENFI, symptomatic participants were older than asymptomatic participants and showed marked deficits in neuropsychological and informant-based assessment of severity.

TABLE 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics for the participants recruited at the Cambridge Centre for Frontotemporal Dementia and Related Disorders
Control (n = 32) FTD (n = 27) Statistic (t2)
Age 67.2 (8.5) 64.3 (7.3) t(57) = 1.4 , P = .16
Sex (M/F) 14/18 17/11 χ = 1.5, P= .23
ACE-R 65 (20)
FAB 9.3 (4.4)
CBI-R 74.3 (22.3)
  • Scores are mean (SD). ACE-R, Addenbrookes Cognitive Examination-Revised; CBI-R, Cambridge Behavioral Inventory-Revised; FAB, Frontal Assessment Battery.
TABLE 2. Demographic and clinical characteristics for the GENFI participants
NC PSC Symp NC vs. Symp NC vs. PSC
n = 309 n = 320 n = 150

Stat (χ2/t)

P d

Stat (χ2/t)

P d
Age (y) 48 (13) 45 (12) 63 (8.2) t = –15 <.0001 1.3 t = 2.5 .01 0.2
Gender (F/M) 179/130 197/123 67/83 Χ = 7 0.008 Χ = 0.9 0.35
Mutation (n)

C9orf72 109

GRN 133

MAPT 60

TBK1 7

C9orf72 119

GRN 141

MAPT 58

TBK1 2

C9orf72 71

GRN 53

MAPT 26

Χ = 5 .06 Χ = 0.3 .9
MMSE 29 (1) 29 (1) 21 (7) t = 13 <.0001 1.9 t = –0.1 .92 0
CBI-R Total 5 (7) 6 (9) 62 (32) t = –21 <.0001 –3 t = –1.5 .1 –0.13
Trail Making Test B 67 (37) 67 (40) 211 (92) t = –16 <.0001 –2.4 t = 0.13 .99 0
Digit Symbol 58 (14) 58 (14) 25 (14) t = 22 <.0001 2.3 t = 0 1 0
Backwards Digit Span score 4.8 (1.2) 4.8 (1.2) 3.1 (1.5) t = 13 <.0001 1.4 t = –0.54 .6 –0.04
Boston Naming 28 (2) 28 (3) 19 (8) t = 13 <.0001 1.8 t = 0.84 .4 0.07
Letter Fluency 41 (13) 41 (13) 17 (12) t = 18 <.0001 1.9 t = 0.84 .4 0.07
Category Fluency 23 (6) 24 (6) 11 (6) t = 20 <.0001 2.2 t = –1.5 .14 –0.12
  • Scores are mean (SD). P values minimum threshold of <.0001.
  • CBI-R, Cambridge Behavioral Inventory-Revised; MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination; NC, non-carrier; PSC, presymptomatic mutation carrier; Symp, symptomatic.

3.2 Network dynamics in FTD

For the Cambridge data, we used temporally concatenated participant time series from ICA components to fit an HMM with six brain states (Figure S1A, with labeling in Figure S2 to indicate the most closely matching canonical static network for positive and negative activations). Participants with FTD had increased fractional occupancy of state 2, whose positive activations constituted the salience network (F = 7.8, P = .043). Switching rates between states were reduced in FTD (Figure S1; F = 6.5, P = .014).

Details are in the caption following the image
Network dynamics in the Genetic Frontotemporal Initiative. (A) Mean activation maps for the six modelled states. (B) Fractional occupancy by state, with increased occupancy in states 2 and 4, and decreased occupancy in states 3 and 5. (C) Altered transition and persistence probabilities in frontotemporal dementia (FTD) using a permutation test. Blue lines represent significantly decreased transitions in FTD, and red lines significantly increased transitions. The figures show the absolute percentage increase or decrease in probability in FTD

For the GENFI data, we used temporally concatenated participant component timeseries to fit an HMM with six brain states (Figure 1A-C and S2). Comparing symptomatic participants with mutation non-carriers, we found that participants with FTD had increased fractional occupancy of the state overlapping with the salience network (state 2, F = 32, corrected P = 2 × 10−7) and of state 4 overlapping with the default mode network (F = 8, P = .008). Participants with FTD spent less time than non-carriers in two states with inversed activation patterns: state 3 with positive activations in sub-cortical regions (F = 17, P = 1 × 10−4), and state 5 with positive activations in motor and sensory (somatic, visual, and auditory) regions (F = 15, corrected P = 2 × 10−4). In this cohort switching rates did not differ in FTD (F = 3.1, P = .08).

We performed a principal component analysis with varimax rotation on state occupancies for each cohort. In the GENFI cohort one component was selected, which explained 68% of the variance (Figure 2A). Higher component scores were associated with greater time in states 2, 4, and 6, and decreased time in states 3 and 5. Component scores were increased in symptomatic participants (F = 21, P = 4 × 10−7). There was a weak trend between component scores and motion assessment indices in symptomatic participants (maximum framewise displacement Pearson's R = 0.047, P = .57; maximum DVARS R = 0.042, P = .61; maximum spike percentage R = 0.1 P = .1). Comparable components were derived for the Cambridge cohort (Supplementary Materials).

Details are in the caption following the image
Changes in network dynamics occurring in the late presymptomatic phase. (A) Component loadings from a principal component analysis (PCA) on state occupancies. (B) Component scores showing a significant increase in converters (at their latest presymptomatic scan) in contrast to those who have not converted to the symptomatic phase during longitudinal follow-up. (C) Fractional occupancy by state, showing an increase for converters in state 2 (salience) occupancy. (D) State 2 occupancy in all carriers. (E) State 2 occupancy in presymptomatic mutation carriers (PSC) showing evidence of a non-linear relationship with age, in contrast to non-carriers (NC). GENFI, Genetic Frontotemporal Initiative; HMM, hidden Markov model

We found no difference in component scores by mutation or clinical phenotype (Supplementary Materials). Component scores were associated with carer-based assessments and neuropsychological scores in symptomatic and presymptomatic mutation carriers (Supplementary Materials and Figure S4).

3.3 Network dynamics in mutation carriers

We investigated temporal dynamics across all mutation carriers. We hypothesized that fractional occupancy would show a non-linear relationship with age, as a proxy marker of proximity to symptom onset. We therefore included a quadratic term for age using orthogonalized polynomials. Model comparison found that inclusion of a quadratic age term to a linear model significantly improved fit for state 2, but not for component scores or other states (Table S2 and Figure 2D).

Within a mixed model including age as a quadratic term and with sex and site as covariates of no interest, state 2 occupancy showed an uncorrected difference between non-carriers and presymptomatic mutation carriers as a function of age (interaction F = 3.8, uncorrected P = .022, Figure 2E), results that were not replicated in a purely linear model (F = 1.7, uncorrected P = .19). No differences were observed for other states or components scores.

3.4 Network dynamics predict symptomatic conversion

Fourteen presymptomatic carriers became symptomatic during follow-up. We compared these converters at their latest presymptomatic visit with imaging with other presymptomatic carriers. Converters had significantly worse performance on neuropsychological assessment at this visit (Backwards Digit Span F = 5.7, P = .017; Backwards Digit Span score F = 6.9, P = .009; Trail Making Test B F = 28, P = 2 × 10−7). We found that component scores and state 2 occupancy were increased in converters (Figure 2B,C).

3.5 Network dynamics predict cognitive decline

We assessed whether higher baseline component scores in symptomatic patients were associated with subsequent neurocognitive decline using pre-registered assessments (TMTB, Digit Symbol, Backwards Digit Span) and measures of global cognitive and behavioral decline (CBI-R, MMSE). Patients at floor scores for assessments were removed prior to deriving linear mixed models (TMTB n = 20, Backwards Digit Span n = 2, Digit Symbol n = 2). Linear mixed models on longitudinal clinical and neurocognitive scores indicated an effect of time for all measures in symptomatic participants (Table S3).

Correcting for age at baseline scan, sex, and site, baseline component scores were related to the annual rate of clinical progression for MMSE (Figure 3A, Std Beta = −0.43, P = .001). The associations with Backwards Digit Span (Std Beta = −0.26, uncorrected P = .021, P = .054) and TMTB (Std Beta = 0.35, uncorrected P = .035, P = .059) were not significant after correction for multiple comparisons. No significant relationship was found with Digit Symbol (Std Beta = −0.21, P = .089) or carer-rated severity using the CBI-R (Std Beta = 0.16 P = .18). We found a significant difference in slope between symptomatic mutation carriers and non-carriers for MMSE, TMTB, and CBI-R (group × baseline component score interaction: MMSE Std Beta = −0.66, P = 2 × 10−10; Backwards Digit Span Std Beta = −0.23 P = .11; Digit Symbol Std Beta = −0.12 P = .18; TMTB Std Beta = 0.58 P = 5 × 10−5; CBI-R Std Beta = 0.12 P = .041).

Details are in the caption following the image
Cognitive decline in symptomatic participants. (A) Baseline component scores predict subsequent cognitive decline in symptomatic participants in the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE), with an uncorrected association with Digit Span and Trail Making Test B (TMTB). Annualized rates of change in cognitive scores are derived from a mixed linear effect model, and taken to a second model to compare with component scores while partialing out covariates. (B) Baseline state 2 occupancy predicts subsequent cognitive decline in symptomatic patients in a range of clinical and neuropsychological tests. CBI-R, Cambridge Behavioral Inventory-Revised

We proceeded to investigate whether baseline network dynamics predicted cognitive and clinical decline in presymptomatic mutation carriers, hypothesizing that the relationship between annualized rate of change in neurocognitive measure and component scores would depend on age as a marker of proximity to symptom onset.

We found that age significantly modified the relationship between annualized rate of clinical progression and baseline component scores for TMTB (interaction Std Beta = 0.21 P = .002), and MMSE (interaction Std Beta = −0.14 P = .048). For the TMTB, a significant three-way interaction (group × age × component score) implied that baseline component score increased the rate of clinical deterioration in older presymptomatic mutation carriers, relative to non-carriers or younger carriers (Table 3). We did not find any significant relationships with Digit Symbol, Backwards Digit Span, or CBI-R.

TABLE 3. Two-step prediction models for presymptomatic mutation carriers
Slope ∼ comp + cov Slope ∼ comp*age + cov Slope ∼ comp*age*group + cov
Model Std Beta t P Std Beta t P Std Beta t P
TMTB –0.13 –1.0 .75 0.22 3.9 .0006 0.43 5.1 2x10-6
Digit Span 0.02 0.37 .75 0.09 1.5 .17 0.12 1.3 .19
Digit Symbol –0.02 –0.31 .75 –0.02 –0.45 .66 –0.12 –1.6 .15
MMSE –0.05 –0.76 .75 –0.14 –2.4 .048 –0.19 –2.1 .072
CBI-R 0.03 0.50 .75 0.11 1.7 .15 0.20 2.0 .072
  • comp, fractional occupancy component; CBI-R, Cambridge Behavioral Inventory-Revised; MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination; TMTB, Trail Making Test B.

Given the difference in state 2 occupancies both in converters and between non-carriers and presymptomatic mutation carriers, together with the known role of the salience network in FTD, we also investigated the relationship between baseline state 2 occupancy and longitudinal cognitive decline. We found baseline salience state occupancy predicted cognitive decline in symptomatic carriers in all measures except CBI-R, and for the TMTB, MMSE, and CBI-R in older presymptomatic mutation carriers (Figure 3B and supplementary materials).

4 DISCUSSION

This study demonstrates that the temporal dynamics of large-scale brain networks are disrupted by sporadic and familial FTD, with characteristic changes in both the symptomatic and late presymptomatic phases of disease. There is an increase in salience and default mode network occupancy, and a decrease in proportion of time spent in the primary cortices and subcortical regions: a change which correlates with clinical and neuropsychological markers of disease severity. Changes in temporal dynamics occur near to disease onset and predict the onset and deterioration of the clinical syndrome as evidenced by (1) the increased component scores of those who subsequently converted to the symptomatic phase during follow-up, and (2) increased rates of cognitive and clinical decline in both symptomatic and older presymptomatic participants with higher component scores.

Functional networks provide an intermediate phenotype to investigate the compensatory changes that account for the dissociation between neuropathological progression and maintained cognitive performance in presymptomatic neurodegeneration,40 with coupling between functional connectivity and cognition increasing close to disease onset.4, 41 Changes in time-varying connectivity predict behavioral traits beyond static functional connectivity or structure alone,11, 31 suggesting that investigating network dynamics informs our understanding of the transition from the presymptomatic to symptomatic phase of neurodegenerative disease. Here we found that while the dynamic repertoire is unchanged through much of the presymptomatic period, the onset of change indicates future symptomatic decline. This suggests that network dynamics can potentially be used both to guide prognosis and as an intermediate marker of success for interventions in presymptomatic mutation carriers, adding to existing clinical, blood, and other imaging biomarkers.42

Given that the salience network is selectively targeted in bvFTD, with atrophy of network hubs and reduced functional connectivity,43-45 the finding of increased salience network occupancy in FTD in both cohorts is perhaps unexpected. The salience network is integral to accessing other large-scale networks, including executive46 and default mode networks.47 Neuropathological disruption to salience network connectivity may undermine its ability to coordinate network switching, perturbing global network dynamics, resulting in increased time spent in a state with positive activations in the default mode network and increased time within the salience network itself. Assessment of between-group differences in transition probabilities provides a potential explanation for these changes. We found a reduced frequency of transition from the salience state to the subcortical (primarily thalamic) state. Subcortical atrophy is well recognized in FTD, notably in the thalamus, and occurs in both sporadic and genetic FTD,48, 49 including in the presymptomatic phase.1 Our findings could suggest that subcortical network integrity influences cortical salience network dynamics, echoing previous work describing the role of thalamic degeneration in disrupting salience network connectivity in genetic FTD.48

There are limitations to our study, despite the advantages of cross-sectional replication and longitudinal follow-up in the GENFI data. The HMM provides a data-driven explanation of the data without biological assumptions,50 with resulting constraints to its explanatory power. It is possible that a time-varying connectivity approach with additional biologically informed constraints could provide further group differentiation and refined longitudinal predictions. Our approach was not optimized to find differences in brain state dynamics between mutation types or by phenotype. Alternative methodological choices may reveal such differences, according to different a priori numbers of states, focusing on different large-scale networks and modelling subsets of patients. In the GENFI cohort the study design necessarily results in a significant age difference between non-carriers and symptomatic participants. That similar patterns of state occupancies were observed in the Cambridge cohort suggests that our results are not primarily driven by age differences.

We conclude that network dynamics are a critical link between neuropathology and symptomatology, heralding symptom onset and correlating with key measures of clinical severity. Network dynamics are a promising tool for stratification and prognostication in FTD.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

We thank our participant volunteers and their families for their participation, and the radiographers and research nurses from all centers involved in this study for their invaluable support. GENFI was funded by the Medical Research Council UK (MR/M023664/1), the Bluefield Project, the JPND GENFI-PROX grant (by DLR/BMBF 2019-02248) and by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation) under Germany's Excellence Strategy within the framework of the Munich Cluster for Systems Neurology (EXC 2145 SyNergy – ID 390857198). The study was co-funded by the National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Biomedical Research Centre at Cambridge University Hospitals NHS Foundation Trust and the University of Cambridge (BRC-1215-20014), the Cambridge Centre for Parkinson-plus (RG95450); the Wellcome Trust (220258); the Evelyn Trust (17/09) and Medical Research Council (SUAG/092 116768). The views expressed are those of the authors and not necessarily those of the NIHR or the Department of Health and Social Care. For the purpose of open access, the authors have applied a CC-BY public copyright license to any Author Accepted Manuscript version arising from this submission.

    CONFLICT OF INTEREST

    James B. Rowe is a non-remunerated trustee of the Guarantors of Brain, Darwin College, and the PSP Association; he provides consultancy to Alzheimer Research UK, Asceneuron, Biogen, CuraSen, UCB, SV Health, and Wave, and has research grants from AZ-Medimmune, Janssen, Lilly as industry partners in the Dementias Platform UK. Johannes Levin reports speaker fees from Bayer Vital, Biogen, and Roche, consulting fees from Axon Neuroscience and Biogen, author fees from Thieme medical publishers and W. Kohlhammer GmbH medical publishers, non-financial support from AbbVie and compensation for duty as part-time CMO from MODAG. Author disclosures are available in the supporting information.

    APPENDIX

    GENFI consortium authors

    Aitana Sogorb Esteve (Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, Dementia Research Centre, UCL Queen Square Institute of Neurology, London, UK; UK Dementia Research Institute at University College London, UCL Queen Square Institute of Neurology, London, UK)
    Annabel Nelson (Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, Dementia Research Centre, UCL Queen Square Institute of Neurology, London, UK)
    Arabella Bouzigues (Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, Dementia Research Centre, UCL Queen Square Institute of Neurology, London, UK)
    Carolin Heller (Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, Dementia Research Centre, UCL Queen Square Institute of Neurology, London, UK)
    Caroline V Greaves (Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, Dementia Research Centre, UCL Queen Square Institute of Neurology, London, UK)
    David Cash (Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, Dementia Research Centre, UCL Queen Square Institute of Neurology, London, UK)
    David L Thomas (Neuroimaging Analysis Centre, Department of Brain Repair and Rehabilitation, UCL Institute of Neurology, Queen Square, London, UK)
    Emily Todd (Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, Dementia Research Centre, UCL Queen Square Institute of Neurology, London, UK)
    Hanya Benotmane (UK Dementia Research Institute at University College London, UCL Queen Square Institute of Neurology, London, UK)
    Henrik Zetterberg (UK Dementia Research Institute at University College London, UCL Queen Square Institute of Neurology, London, UK Department of Psychiatry and Neurochemistry,; the Sahlgrenska Academy at the University of Gothenburg, Mölndal, Sweden)
    Imogen J Swift (Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, Dementia Research Centre, UCL Queen Square Institute of Neurology, London, UK; UK Dementia Research Institute at University College London, UCL Queen Square Institute of Neurology, London, UK)
    Jennifer Nicholas (Department of Medical Statistics, London School of Hygiene and Tropical Medicine, London, UK)
    Kiran Samra (Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, Dementia Research Centre, UCL Queen Square Institute of Neurology, London, UK)
    Lucy L Russell (Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, Dementia Research Centre, UCL Queen Square Institute of Neurology, London, UK)
    Martina Bocchetta (Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, Dementia Research Centre, UCL Queen Square Institute of Neurology, London, UK)
    Rachelle Shafei (Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, Dementia Research Centre, UCL Queen Square Institute of Neurology, London, UK)
    Rhian S Convery (Department of Neurodegenerative Disease, Dementia Research Centre, UCL Queen Square Institute of Neurology, London, UK)
    Carolyn Timberlake (Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK)
    Thomas Cope (Department of Clinical Neuroscience, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK)
    Timothy Rittman (Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, UK)
    Alberto Benussi (Centre for Neurodegenerative Disorders, Department of Clinical and Experimental Sciences, University of Brescia, Brescia, Italy)
    Enrico Premi (Stroke Unit, ASST Brescia Hospital, Brescia, Italy)
    Roberto Gasparotti (Neuroradiology Unit, University of Brescia, Brescia, Italy)
    Silvana Archetti (Biotechnology Laboratory, Department of Diagnostics, ASST Brescia Hospital, Brescia, Italy)
    Stefano Gazzina (Neurology, ASST Brescia Hospital, Brescia, Italy)
    Valentina Cantoni (Centre for Neurodegenerative Disorders, Department of Clinical and Experimental Sciences, University of Brescia, Brescia, Italy)
    Andrea Arighi (Fondazione IRCCS Ca’ Granda Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, Neurodegenerative Diseases Unit, Milan, Italy; University of Milan, Centro Dino Ferrari, Milan, Italy)
    Chiara Fenoglio (Fondazione IRCCS Ca’ Granda Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, Neurodegenerative Diseases Unit, Milan, Italy; University of Milan, Centro Dino Ferrari, Milan, Italy)
    Elio Scarpini (Fondazione IRCCS Ca’ Granda Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, Neurodegenerative Diseases Unit, Milan, Italy; University of Milan, Centro Dino Ferrari, Milan, Italy)
    Giorgio Fumagalli (Fondazione IRCCS Ca’ Granda Ospedale Maggiore Policlinico, Neurodegenerative Diseases Unit, Milan, Italy; University of Milan, Centro Dino Ferrari, Milan, Italy)
    Vittoria Borracci
    Giacomina Rossi (Fondazione IRCCS Istituto Neurologico Carlo Besta, Milano, Italy)
    Giorgio Giaccone (Fondazione IRCCS Istituto Neurologico Carlo Besta, Milano, Italy)
    Giuseppe Di Fede (Fondazione IRCCS Istituto Neurologico Carlo Besta, Milano, Italy)
    Paola Caroppo (Fondazione IRCCS Istituto Neurologico Carlo Besta, Milano, Italy)
    Pietro Tiraboschi (Fondazione IRCCS Istituto Neurologico Carlo Besta, Milano, Italy)
    Sara Prioni (Fondazione IRCCS Istituto Neurologico Carlo Besta, Milano, Italy)
    Veronica Redaelli (Fondazione IRCCS Istituto Neurologico Carlo Besta, Milano, Italy)
    David Tang-Wai (The University Health Network, Krembil Research Institute, Toronto, Canada)
    Ekaterina Rogaeva (Tanz Centre for Research in Neurodegenerative Diseases, University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada)
    Miguel Castelo-Branco (Faculty of Medicine, University of Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal)
    Morris Freedman (Baycrest Health Sciences, Rotman Research Institute, University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada)
    Ron Keren (The University Health Network, Toronto Rehabilitation Institute, Toronto, Canada)
    Sandra Black (Sunnybrook Health Sciences Centre, Sunnybrook Research Institute, University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada)
    Sara Mitchell (Sunnybrook Health Sciences Centre, Sunnybrook Research Institute, University of Toronto, Toronto, Canada)
    Christen Shoesmith (Department of Clinical Neurological Sciences, University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada)
    Robart Bartha (Department of Medical Biophysics, The University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada; Centre for Functional and Metabolic Mapping, Robarts Research Institute, The University of Western Ontario, London, Ontario, Canada)
    Rosa Rademakers (Center for Molecular Neurology, University of Antwerp)
    Jackie Poos (Department of Neurology, Erasmus Medical Center, Rotterdam, Netherlands)
    Janne M. Papma (Department of Neurology, Erasmus Medical Center, Rotterdam, Netherlands)
    Lucia Giannini (Department of Neurology, Erasmus Medical Center, Rotterdam, Netherlands)
    Rick van Minkelen (Department of Clinical Genetics, Erasmus Medical Center, Rotterdam, Netherlands)
    Yolande Pijnenburg (Amsterdam University Medical Centre, Amsterdam VUmc, Amsterdam, Netherlands)
    Camilla Ferrari (Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Drug Research and Child Health, University of Florence, Florence, Italy)
    Cristina Polito (Department of Biomedical, Experimental and Clinical Sciences “Mario Serio”, Nuclear Medicine Unit, University of Florence, Florence, Italy)
    Gemma Lombardi (Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Drug Research and Child Health, University of Florence, Florence, Italy)
    Valentina Bessi (Department of Neuroscience, Psychology, Drug Research and Child Health, University of Florence, Florence, Italy)
    Michele Veldsman (Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, Medical Sciences Division, University of Oxford, Oxford, UK)
    Christin Andersson (Department of Clinical Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden)
    Hakan Thonberg (Center for Alzheimer Research, Division of Neurogeriatrics, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden)
    Linn Öijerstedt (Center for Alzheimer Research, Division of Neurogeriatrics, Department of Neurobiology, Care Sciences and Society, Bioclinicum, Karolinska Institutet, Solna, Sweden; Unit for Hereditary Dementias, Theme Aging, Karolinska University Hospital, Solna, Sweden)
    Vesna Jelic (Division of Clinical Geriatrics, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden)
    Paul Thompson (Division of Neuroscience and Experimental Psychology, Wolfson Molecular Imaging Centre, University of Manchester, Manchester, UK)
    Tobias Langheinrich (Division of Neuroscience and Experimental Psychology, Wolfson Molecular Imaging Centre, University of Manchester, Manchester, UK Manchester Centre for Clinical Neurosciences, Department of Neurology, Salford Royal NHS Foundation Trust, Manchester, UK)
    Albert Lladó (Alzheimer's disease and Other Cognitive Disorders Unit, Neurology Service, Hospital Clínic, Barcelona, Spain)
    Anna Antonell (Alzheimer's disease and Other Cognitive Disorders Unit, Neurology Service, Hospital Clínic, Barcelona, Spain)
    Jaume Olives (Alzheimer's disease and Other Cognitive Disorders Unit, Neurology Service, Hospital Clínic, Barcelona, Spain)
    Mircea Balasa (Alzheimer's disease and Other Cognitive Disorders Unit, Neurology Service, Hospital Clínic, Barcelona, Spain)
    Nuria Bargalló (Imaging Diagnostic Center, Hospital Clínic, Barcelona, Spain)
    Sergi Borrego-Ecija (Alzheimer's disease and Other Cognitive Disorders Unit, Neurology Service, Hospital Clínic, Barcelona, Spain)
    Ana Verdelho (Department of Neurosciences and Mental Health, Centro Hospitalar Lisboa Norte - Hospital de Santa Maria & Faculty of Medicine, University of Lisbon, Lisbon, Portugal)
    Carolina Maruta (Laboratory of Language Research, Centro de Estudos Egas Moniz, Faculty of Medicine, University of Lisbon, Lisbon, Portugal)
    Catarina B. Ferreira (Laboratory of Neurosciences, Faculty of Medicine, University of Lisbon, Lisbon, Portugal)
    Gabriel Miltenberger (Faculty of Medicine, University of Lisbon, Lisbon, Portugal)
    Frederico Simões do Couto (Faculdade de Medicina, Universidade Católica Portuguesa)
    Alazne Gabilondo (Cognitive Disorders Unit, Department of Neurology, Donostia University Hospital, San Sebastian, Gipuzkoa, Spain; Neuroscience Area, Biodonostia Health Research Institute, San Sebastian, Gipuzkoa, Spain)
    Ana Gorostidi (Neuroscience Area, Biodonostia Health Research Institute, San Sebastian, Gipuzkoa, Spain)
    Jorge Villanua (OSATEK, University of Donostia, San Sebastian, Gipuzkoa, Spain)
    Marta Cañada (CITA Alzheimer, San Sebastian, Gipuzkoa, Spain)
    Mikel Tainta (Neuroscience Area, Biodonostia Health Research Institute, San Sebastian, Gipuzkoa, Spain)
    Miren Zulaica (Neuroscience Area, Biodonostia Health Research Institute, San Sebastian, Gipuzkoa, Spain)
    Myriam Barandiaran (Cognitive Disorders Unit, Department of Neurology, Donostia University Hospital, San Sebastian, Gipuzkoa, Spain; Neuroscience Area, Biodonostia Health Research Institute, San Sebastian, Gipuzkoa, Spain)
    Patricia Alves (Neuroscience Area, Biodonostia Health Research Institute, San Sebastian, Gipuzkoa, Spain; Department of Educational Psychology and Psychobiology, Faculty of Education, International University of La Rioja, Logroño, Spain)
    Benjamin Bender (Department of Diagnostic and Interventional Neuroradiology, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany)
    Carlo Wilke (Department of Neurodegenerative Diseases, Hertie-Institute for Clinical Brain Research and Center of Neurology, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany; Center for Neurodegenerative Diseases (DZNE), Tübingen, Germany)
    Lisa Graf (Department of Neurodegenerative Diseases, Hertie-Institute for Clinical Brain Research and Center of Neurology, University of Tübingen, Tübingen, Germany)
    Annick Vogels (Department of Human Genetics, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium)
    Mathieu Vandenbulcke (Geriatric Psychiatry Service, University Hospitals Leuven, Belgium; Neuropsychiatry, Department of Neurosciences, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium)
    Philip Van Damme (Neurology Service, University Hospitals Leuven, Belgium; Laboratory for Neurobiology, VIB-KU Leuven Centre for Brain Research, Leuven, Belgium)
    Rose Bruffaerts (Department of Biomedical Sciences, University of Antwerp, Antwerp, Belgium; Biomedical Research Institute, Hasselt University, 3500 Hasselt, Belgium)
    Koen Poesen (Laboratory for Molecular Neurobiomarker Research, KU Leuven, Leuven, Belgium)
    Pedro Rosa-Neto (Translational Neuroimaging Laboratory, McGill Centre for Studies in Aging, McGill University, Montreal, Québec, Canada)

    Serge Gauthier (Alzheimer Disease Research Unit, McGill Centre for Studies in Aging, Department of Neurology & Neurosurgery, McGill University, Montreal, Québec, Canada)

    Agnès Camuzat (Sorbonne Université, Paris Brain Institute – Institut du Cerveau – ICM, Inserm U1127, CNRS UMR 7225, AP-HP - Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Paris, France)
    Alexis Brice (Sorbonne Université, Paris Brain Institute – Institut du Cerveau – ICM, Inserm U1127, CNRS UMR 7225, AP-HP - Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Paris, France; Reference Network for Rare Neurological Diseases (ERN-RND))
    Anne Bertrand (Sorbonne Université, Paris Brain Institute – Institut du Cerveau – ICM, Inserm U1127, CNRS UMR 7225, AP-HP - Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Paris, FranceInria, Aramis project-team, F-75013, Paris, France; Centre pour l'Acquisition et le Traitement des Images, Institut du Cerveau et la Moelle, Paris, France)
    Aurélie Funkiewiez (Centre de référence des démences rares ou précoces, IM2A, Département de Neurologie, AP-HP - Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Paris, France; Sorbonne Université, Paris Brain Institute – Institut du Cerveau – ICM, Inserm U1127, CNRS UMR 7225, AP-HP - Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Paris, France)
    Daisy Rinaldi (Centre de référence des démences rares ou précoces, IM2A, Département de Neurologie, AP-HP - Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Paris, France; Sorbonne Université, Paris Brain Institute – Institut du Cerveau – ICM, Inserm U1127, CNRS UMR 7225, AP-HP - Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Paris, France; Département de Neurologie, AP-HP - Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Paris, France)

    Olivier Colliot (Sorbonne Université, Paris Brain Institute – Institut du Cerveau – ICM, Inserm U1127, CNRS UMR 7225, AP-HP - Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Paris, France; Inria, Aramis project-team, F-75013, Paris, France; Centre pour l'Acquisition et le Traitement des Images, Institut du Cerveau et la Moelle, Paris, France)

    Sabrina Sayah (Sorbonne Université, Paris Brain Institute – Institut du Cerveau – ICM, Inserm U1127, CNRS UMR 7225, AP-HP - Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Paris, France)

    Catharina Prix (Neurologische Klinik, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, Munich, Germany)
    Elisabeth Wlasich (Neurologische Klinik, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, Munich, Germany)
    Olivia Wagemann
    Sandra Loosli (Neurologische Klinik, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, Munich, Germany)
    Sonja Schönecker (Neurologische Klinik, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, Munich, Germany)
    Tobias Hoegen (Neurologische Klinik, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, Munich, Germany)
    Jolina Lombardi (Department of Neurology, University of Ulm, Ulm)
    Sarah Anderl-Straub (Department of Neurology, University of Ulm, Ulm, Germany)
    Adeline Rollin (CHU, CNR-MAJ, Labex Distalz, LiCEND Lille, France)
    Gregory Kuchcinski (Univ Lille, FranceInserm 1172, Lille, France; CHU, CNR-MAJ, Labex Distalz, LiCEND Lille, France)
    Maxime Bertoux (Inserm 1172, Lille, FranceCHU, CNR-MAJ, Labex Distalz, LiCEND Lille, France)
    Thibaud Lebouvier (Univ Lille, France; Inserm 1172, Lille, France; CHU, CNR-MAJ, Labex Distalz, LiCEND Lille, France)
    Vincent Deramecourt (Univ Lille, France; Inserm 1172, Lille, France; CHU, CNR-MAJ, Labex Distalz, LiCEND Lille, France)
    Beatriz Santiago (Neurology Department, Centro Hospitalar e Universitario de Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal)
    Diana Duro (Faculty of Medicine, University of Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal)
    Maria João Leitão (Centre of Neurosciences and Cell Biology, Universidade de Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal)
    Maria Rosario Almeida (Faculty of Medicine, University of Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal)
    Miguel Tábuas-Pereira (Neurology Department, Centro Hospitalar e Universitario de Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal)
    Sónia Afonso (Instituto Ciencias Nucleares Aplicadas a Saude, Universidade de Coimbra, Coimbra, Portugal)
    Annerose Engel (Clinic for Cognitive Neurology, University Hospital Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany)
    Maryna Polyakova (Department for Neurology, Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences and Clinic for Cognitive Neurology, University Hospital Leipzig, Leipzig, Germany)